Published on in Vol 6 (2023)

This is a member publication of University of Sheffield (Jisc)

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/45368, first published .
Investigating the Role of Upward Comparisons and Self-compassion on Stigma in People With Acne: Cross-sectional Study

Investigating the Role of Upward Comparisons and Self-compassion on Stigma in People With Acne: Cross-sectional Study

Investigating the Role of Upward Comparisons and Self-compassion on Stigma in People With Acne: Cross-sectional Study

Original Paper

1Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom

2Department of History, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom

Corresponding Author:

Paul G Overton, BA, PhD

Department of Psychology

University of Sheffield

Western Bank

Sheffield, S10 2TN

United Kingdom

Phone: 44 7851010863

Email: p.g.overton@sheffield.ac.uk


Background: The use of image-laden social media is hypothesized as being implicated in psychological distress in individuals with conditions affecting their appearance. However, relatively little is known about the mechanisms involved in this relationship.

Objective: This cross-sectional study examined the relationship between photo-orientated social media use and feelings of stigmatization in adults with acne, and tested whether upward skin comparisons mediate and self-compassion moderates this relationship.

Methods: Adults (N=650) with acne symptoms completed web-based measures of social media use (daily Facebook or Instagram use, Facebook function use), self-compassion, skin appearance comparisons, and internalized stigmatization.

Results: Moderated-mediation and mediation analyses indicated that there was a significant indirect effect of Facebook photo use on stigmatization, mediated by upward appearance comparisons (estimation of indirect effect 11.03, SE 5.11, 95% CI 1.19-21.12). There was no significant relationship between Instagram use and feelings of stigmatization (estimation of indirect effect 0.0002, SE 0.005, 95% CI −0.011 to 0.009), yet upward appearance comparisons predicted feelings of stigmatization (B=0.99, P<.001). Self-compassion did not moderate the indirect or direct relationships between photo-orientated social media use and stigma. However, self-compassion was negatively correlated with upward appearance comparisons and feelings of stigmatization in both Facebook and Instagram users.

Conclusions: The way that individuals engage with social media, and in particular make appearance comparisons, should be considered when working with individuals with skin-related distress. Interventions aimed at boosting self-compassion and reducing appearance comparisons may provide avenues for protecting against feelings of stigma.

JMIR Dermatol 2023;6:e45368

doi:10.2196/45368

Keywords



Individuals living with visible skin conditions, including acne vulgaris (acne), can experience stigmatization from others (enacted stigma) and internalize feelings of stigmatization (felt stigma) [1-9]. Individual accounts of stigma [2,4,5] are corroborated by experimental research that indicates an implicit preference for clear skin and negative assumptions about individuals with acne [6-8]. Stigma has important implications for psychosocial well-being; surveys of individuals with acne report that felt stigma explains the largest variance (25%-36%), beyond other predictors (eg, perceived severity and gender), across quality-of-life domains: self-perception, social, and emotional [1,9]. Similar findings have been reported in populations with other skin conditions [10,11].

The psychological burden of acne has been well documented [12]. Of the various domains of impact, the impact of acne on self-perception has been the most widely reported. So, acne leads to appearance-related distress [13], body image disturbance [14], and feeling unattractive or ugly [15]. In order to reduce the psychological burden associated with acne, it is essential to understand the psychological mechanisms involved in acne stigmatization. Understanding these mechanisms using psychological theory has the potential to provide theoretical “proof of concept” for suitable targets for psychological therapy. Although dermatological (severity and duration) [1,16,17] and demographic (employment status, age, relationship status, and gender) [1,16-20] variables have limited predictive power, wider sociocultural factors are likely to play a more significant role. Sociocultural factors, including contemporary media, are theorized to influence societal norms and appearance ideals, contributing to the stigmatization of individuals who are unable to meet these ideals [21-23]. Correspondingly, within a qualitative study, participants with acne, eczema, and psoriasis described a pervasive media ideal of perfect skin [24]. Failure to meet this ideal was related to greater depression and stigmatization in female, but not male participants [24]. However, there was no distinction between media platforms or investigation of the specific psychological mechanisms that might be involved.

Web-based activity now plays a major role in our lives. As of 2021, a total of 88% of all UK adults possessed a social media account [25]. Facebook remains the most popular site (66% of adult social media users report using Facebook) [25]. Instagram, an image-based platform allowing users to digitally manipulate and share images, is growing in popularity (48% of adult social media users) [25]. Acne frequently affects adolescents [26], a group who are particularly engaged with social media [27].

A number of studies have established a relationship between Facebook use and psychosocial outcomes [28], with the role of individual difference variables in social media use and behavior showing greater promise in explaining the impact of such media, over and above simple usage [29-31]. For example, higher photo-function use, over and above total Facebook usage, has been reported to predict greater weight dissatisfaction, thin-ideal internalization, appearance comparison, and self-objectification [30]. Similar findings are emerging for Instagram use, with undergraduate students experimentally exposed to idealized Instagram images of celebrities and peers, as opposed to Instagram travel pictures, reporting increased body dissatisfaction and negative mood, mediated by appearance comparisons [32].

Early theories of social comparison posited that humans have an innate drive to compare themselves with others as part of maintaining group relationships [33]. As such, a perceived sense of difference may act as a threat, which may drive unhelpful comparisons. Social comparison theory has been expanded to include appearance-based comparisons, and downward and upward comparisons, where individuals compare themselves with others they perceive as superior (upward) or inferior (downward) [34]. Social and upward appearance comparisons have been established as predictors of body dissatisfaction [35], body-shaming [36], and mediators between media exposure to idealized images and body dissatisfaction [30,31,37-39]. Social comparisons are reported to be an important mechanism in the way individuals with a stigmatized identity evaluate themselves [40] and have been theorized as a core process implicated in skin-shaming [41]. Further, Kellett and Gibert [41] have anecdotally found that patients they treat who are distressed in relation to the appearance of their skin condition are often engaging in making such comparisons. However, the relationship between skin-specific appearance comparisons, social media use, and felt stigmatization has thus far not been investigated, nor has the related role of protective factors like self-compassion.

Self-compassion is theorized to involve 3 main components that influence how we treat ourselves and react to difficulties: self-kindness, mindfulness, and common humanity [42]. Self-compassion may act as a protective factor against psychosocial distress in stigmatized populations [43,44], and intervention-based studies using compassion-based training have shown promise in reducing feelings of shame in participants with acne [45].

As a consequence of the lack of research on social media and acne stigmatization, we conducted a web-based survey to investigate the relationship between photo-related social media use and felt stigma in people with acne. We hypothesized that relative photo-based social media use (Facebook photo activity and total time on Instagram), not total time on Facebook, would be related to felt stigmatization: (1) individuals who spend proportionally more of their time using photo- or appearance-orientated social media will have higher levels of felt stigmatization, (2) this relationship will be mediated by upward skin appearance comparisons, and (3) these relationships will be moderated by self-compassion.


Ethical Approval

Ethical approval for this cross-sectional study was granted by the University of Sheffield ethics committee (reference 011937).

Sample and Recruitment

Participants with acne symptoms were recruited between February and March 2017 from a convenience community sample and offered entry into a prize draw. The study was advertised across multiple social media platforms, UK skin charities, web-based recruitment platforms, university volunteer lists, and an undergraduate credit system. To be included in the study, participants were required to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) 16 years or older, (2) current symptoms of acne, (3) living in the United Kingdom or have UK citizenship, and (4) know sufficient English to complete the survey.

A power analysis for multiple regression with 10 predictors indicated that at least 253 participants would be needed to achieve 80% power with a significance level of .05 to detect a small effect size, r=0.25.

Procedure

Participants completed counterbalanced self-report measures of demographics, acne history, Facebook use, Facebook function use, Instagram use, skin-related upward or downward comparisons, self-compassion, and acne stigma via a web-based survey using Qualtrics.

Measures

Demographics and Acne History

Participants provided information about their gender, age, ethnicity, educational level and relationship status, and their acne history, including perceived severity, location of symptoms (categorized as visible and nonvisible), and whether they had received a formal diagnosis or acne treatment from a health professional. Perceived severity was measured using a question based on the fifth question of the Cardiff Acne Disability Index [46], which includes a question about the degree to which acne is a problem for the participant.

Facebook Use

Participants were asked whether they had used Facebook within the past month. If participants answered “yes,” they were asked to estimate the amount of time they spent on Facebook in the past week. Daily Facebook use was calculated using the following formula: Average daily Facebook use = (Number of days Facebook used × Time spent on Facebook on these days) / 7.

Relative Facebook Photo Activity

The Facebook Questionnaire functions [30] assessed relative levels of photo activity compared with nonphoto activities on Facebook. The scale consists of 24 items (α=.86), scored on a 6-point Likert scale. The appearance- or photo-activity subscale is formed of 8 items (α=.76) related to appearance-specific photo activity. Proportionate Facebook photo activity was calculated by dividing the total for the photo-activity subscale by the total for all items. Scores range from 0 to 1; scores closer to 1 indicate a higher proportion of time spent using photo-related functions on Facebook.

Instagram Use

Instagram is an image-based platform. Instagram photo activity was measured using the average time spent on Instagram per day. Participants were asked whether they had used Instagram in the past month. If participants answered “yes,” they were asked to estimate the amount of time they spend on Instagram. Daily Instagram use was calculated using the following formula: Average daily Instagram use = (Number of days Instagram used × Time spent on Instagram on these days) / 7.

Skin-Based Comparisons

The Upward and Downward Appearance Comparison Scales (UPACS, DACS) [34] measure both upward and downward appearance-based comparisons in relation to shape and size. The UPACS and DACS were adapted to measure skin comparisons, and one social media question was added each to the UPACS (“On social media I tend to compare how my skin looks to photographs of people with clearer skin than me”) and the DACS (“On social media I tend to compare how my skin looks to photographs of people with worse skin than me”). Both items correlated highly with the other items in the scales and did not reduce reliability. The adapted UPACS and DACS each contained 9 items (UPACS: α=.93; DACS: α=.94), scored on a 5-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicated higher levels of upward appearance comparisons and higher levels of downward appearance comparisons.

Self-compassion

Self-compassion was measured using the Self-Compassion Scale Short Form [47]. The scale comprises 3 domains: self-kindness versus self-judgment, common humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness versus overidentification. The items (α=.86) are scored on a 5-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicate greater self-compassion.

Skin-Related Stigma

Stigma was measured using the total score on the Feelings of Stigmatization Questionnaire [13], originally developed to assess felt stigmatization in patients with psoriasis. The scale has been adapted and previously used in the context of acne [1]. Amendments to the scale for this study involved replacing the term “psoriasis” with “acne” and replacing the term “patient” with “person” as the survey uses a community sample. One question unrelated to acne (“I do not mind when a family member gives me a vacuum cleaner to clean up the scales that fall from my psoriatic skin”) was deleted. The adapted measure contained 32 questions (α=.92), scored on a 6-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicate greater felt stigmatization.

Analytic Strategy

Data were analyzed using SPSS version 23 (IBM Corp).

Descriptive statistics were calculated using percentages for categorical variables and means and SDs for continuous variables. Demographic and acne history variables were assessed for covariance with felt stigma using t tests, ANOVAs, and bivariate correlations as appropriate. Relationships between the predictor variables, the mediator variables, and the outcome variables were initially tested using bivariate correlations. Nonparametric tests were used when analyzing average Facebook and Instagram use, as normality tests indicated that they were nonnormally distributed. Significant covariates were entered into subsequent analyses.

Hypothesized relationships between photo-related social media activity, upward appearance comparisons, self-compassion, and stigmatization were tested using (1) moderated-mediation (Figures 1 and 2) and (2) mediation-only analysis (Figures 3 and 4), using ordinary least-squares path analysis. Analyses were conducted using the PROCESS macro version 3.442 with 10,000 bootstrap samples.

Figure 1. Moderated-mediation model for Facebook photo use on acne stigma via upward appearance comparison, with self-compassion as the moderator for each path (N=591). The numbers presented in the figure represent unstandardized β values, as recommended by Hayes [48]. The numbers on the arrows intercepting paths a, b, and c represent the unstandardized β values for the interaction effects. (A) P=.03, (B) P<.001, (C) P=.20, (D) P=.25, (E) P=.42. For clarity, covariates are not included in the figure. The covariates that were controlled for on each pathway were gender, severity, acne diagnosis, and downward skin comparison.
Figure 2. Moderated-mediation model for average Instagram use on acne stigma via upward appearance comparison, with self-compassion as the moderator for each path (N=429). The numbers presented in the figure represent unstandardized β values, as recommended by Hayes [48]. The numbers on the arrows intercepting paths a, b, and c represent the unstandardized β values for the interaction effects. (A) P=.98, (B) P<.001, (C) P=.98, (D) P=.25, (E) P=.83. For clarity, covariates are not included in the figure. The covariates that were controlled for on each pathway were gender, severity, acne diagnosis, and downward skin comparison.
Figure 3. Mediation model for Facebook photo use on acne stigma via upward appearance comparison (N=591). The numbers presented in the figure represent unstandardized β values, as recommended by Hayes [48]. (A) P=.03, (B) P<.001, (C) P=.29, (D) 95% CI –11.86 to 39.93. For clarity, covariates are not included in the figure. The covariates that were controlled for on each pathway were gender, severity, acne diagnosis, downward skin comparison, and self-compassion.
Figure 4. Mediation model for average Instagram use on acne stigma via upward appearance comparison (n=429). The numbers presented in the figure represent unstandardized β values, as recommended by Hayes [48]. (A) P=.98, (B) P<.001, (C) P=.53, (D) 95% CI –0.011 to 0.009. For clarity, covariates are not included in the figure. The covariates that were controlled for on each pathway were gender, severity, acne diagnosis, downward skin comparison, and self-compassion.

Demographics

Of 818 participants who started the survey, 652 participants completed the survey. Two participants were excluded because of unfeasible social media use (≥24 hours per day), so overall, 650 participants (aged 16-56 years; 82.9% female) were included in the analyses (69.7% completion rate). Information on participant demographics and acne history is presented in Tables 1 and 2.

In all, 591 participants reported using Facebook, whereas 428 reported using Instagram. The 2 groups were similar in age (Facebook: mean 24.2, SD 6.4 years; Instagram: mean 23.2, SD 5.7 years). Also, there was no association between the 2 platforms (Facebook and Instagram) in terms of the frequency of participants being a student or not a student (χ21=0.21, P=.65), or White or not White (χ21=0.82, P=.37). However, there were a greater proportion of females in the Instagram group (female vs male + other, χ21=5.11, P=.04) and a higher proportion of people 25 years or older (considered to have adult acne [49]) in the Facebook group (χ21=5.01, P=.03).

Participants with a formal acne diagnosis reported higher stigma (mean 76.43, SD 25.22) compared with participants reporting acne symptoms but without a formal diagnosis (mean 67.91, SD 25.73; t648=3.88; P<.001; Cohen d=0.33). Furthermore, Spearman correlations showed a small correlation between self-rated severity and stigmatization (ρ648=0.33, P<.001). However, there was no relationship between the duration of acne symptoms and stigmatization (ρ648=0.06, P=.11).

Free-text responses to the question “Please list any other diagnosed physical or mental health conditions” were coded into yes (1) or no (0) responses on three variables: (1) skin condition, (2) long-term health condition (excluding skin conditions), and (3) mental health conditions. There was no significant effect of other skin conditions on stigmatization (t632=−0.03, P=.98, d=0.003). However, respondents disclosing a comorbid long-term health condition also reported significantly greater acne stigma (mean 78.57, SD 24.04) than participants without a long-term health condition (mean 73.03, SD 25.79; t632=2.15; P=.03; d=0.22). Furthermore, participants disclosing at least 1 diagnosed mental health condition reported significantly higher levels of stigmatization (mean 84.67, SD 25.29) than participants without (mean 71.37, SD 24.92; t632=5.4; P<.001; d=0.53).

Female participants reported higher stigmatization levels (mean 76.17, SD 24.74) compared with male participants (mean 63.34, SD 27.35; t647=0.49; P<.001; d=0.49). No other demographic variables were related to stigma, and those that were (gender, acne diagnosis, acne severity, and long-term health condition) were controlled for in moderated-mediation and mediation analyses. Mental health diagnoses were not included as a covariate as higher levels of mental health problems have previously been identified as a consequence of internalized stigmatization in individuals with skin diseases [10,50].

Table 1. Participant demographics (N=650).
Demographics and participant characteristicsParticipants
Age (years), mean (SD; range)24.47 (6.64; 16-56)

≥25, n (%)225 (34.6)
Gender, n (%)

Female539 (82.9)

Male110 (16.9)

Other1 (0.2)
Ethnicity, n (%)

White or Caucasian510 (78.5)

Asian92 (14.4)

Mixed26 (4.0)

Black10 (1.5)

Arab5 (0.8)

Latin American4 (0.8)

“Prefer not to answer”3 (0.5)
Employment, n (%)

Student409 (62.9)

Employed209 (32.2)

Both employed and student5 (0.8)

Unemployed or unable to work12 (1.8)

Homemakers or carers11 (1.7)

“Prefer not to answer”4 (0.6)
Education level, n (%)

Undergraduate246 (37.8)

A level or equivalent206 (31.7)

Postgraduate145 (22.3)

GCSEa or equivalent21 (3.2)

Vocational22 (3.4)

Other, unsure, or “prefer not to answer”10 (1.5)
Marital status, n (%)

Single285 (43.8)

In a relationship196 (30.2)

Cohabiting with partner84 (12.9)

Married or civil partnership79 (12.2)

“Other” or “prefer not to answer”6 (0.9)

aGCSE: General Certificate of Secondary Education.

Table 2. Participant acne and health history (N=650).
Acne history and participant characteristicsParticipants
Acne duration (months), mean (SD; range)115 (82.38; 1-480)
Acne diagnosis, n (%)

Yes463 (71.2)

No or unsure187 (28.8)
Current treatment, n (%)

GPa189 (29.1)

Dermatologist57 (8.8)

Gynecologist4 (0.6)

Other health professional3 (0.5)

None395 (60.7)

Prefer not to answer2 (0.3)
Locationb, n (%)

Visible638 (98.2)

Nonvisible12 (1.8)
Subjective severity, mean (SD)c2.31 (0.58)
Other diagnoses, n (%)

Yes277 (42.6)

No357 (54.9)

“Prefer not to answer”16 (2.5)

Other skin condition(s)71 (10.9)

Long-term health condition(s)121 (18.6)

Mental health condition(s)129 (19.8)

aGP: general practitioner.

bCharacterized as visible if the location of acne included their face, scalp, neck, hands, or arm.

cRange 1 (not a problem) to 4 (the worst it could be).

Relationships Between Social Media Use, Appearance Comparisons, Self-Compassion, and Stigma

Table 3 provides bivariate correlations for each outcome variable, time spent on Facebook and Instagram, and relative Facebook photo function use.

As predicted, within Facebook users, photo-related Facebook activity positively correlated with upward appearance comparison and stigmatization, whereas average daily Facebook use was not correlated with Facebook photo activity nor stigmatization. Among Instagram users, average time on Instagram correlated positively with upward appearance comparisons but not stigmatization.

Furthermore, among all respondents, there was a large positive correlation between upward comparisons and stigmatization (r648=0.53, P<.001). Self-compassion was negatively correlated with upward comparisons (r648=−0.41, P<.001), stigmatization (r648=−0.46, P<.001), and Facebook photo activity (r590=−0.11, P=.009), but not average Facebook use (ρ590=−0.048, P=.24) nor Instagram use (ρ427=0.093, P=.06).

Downward comparisons had a small significant correlation with Instagram use, upward comparisons, compassion, and stigma, and were therefore included as a covariate within the models below.

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between each of the predictor and outcome variables (N=650).

1, ρ2, r3, ρ4, r5, r6, r7, r
1. FB usea

Coefficient b






P value





2. FB photoa

Coefficient −0.003





P value.94




3. Instagram usec

Coefficient 0.32d0.16d




P value<.001.001



4. UPACSe

Coefficient 0.0540.170.12



P value.19<.001.01


5. DACSf

Coefficient 0.0620.0510.120.38


P value.13.22.01<.001

6. Compassion

Coefficient −0.048−0.11−0.093−0.41−0.18

P value.24.009.06<.001<.001
7. Stigma

Coefficient 0.060.140.0680.530.295−0.46

P value.230.001.162<.001<.001<.001
ValuesMedian 30 (IQR 7.5-52.5; range 0-700)Mean 0.39 (SD 0.069; range 0.00-0.79)Median 30 (IQR 5.0-55.0; range 0-600)Mean 33.49 (SD 8.38; range 9-45)Mean 24.61 (SD 9.01; range 9-45)Mean 32.55 (SD 7.64; range 12-57)Mean 73.98 (SD 25.64; range 7-145)

aExcluding participants who reported not using Facebook (n=592). FB use: average Facebook use per day; FB photo: Facebook Questionnaire functions.

bNot available.

cExcluding participants who reported not using Instagram (n=429). Instagram use: average Instagram use per day.

dExcluding participants who did not use both Facebook and Instagram (n=403)

eUPACS: Upward Appearance Comparison Scale.

fDACS: Downward Appearance Comparison Scale.

Mediation and Moderated-Mediation Analyses

Moderated-mediation analyses were conducted to assess the conditional direct and indirect effects of photo-related social media activity on stigmatization at values of self-compassion 1 SD below the mean, the mean, and 1 SD above the mean.

The results of the moderated-mediation analysis (Figure 1, Table 4) did not support a model of moderated mediation for Facebook photo activity and acne stigma. Interactions of self-compassion on path a (B=0.58, P=.20), path b (B=−0.01, P=.25), or path c (B=−1.07, P=.42) were nonsignificant. Likewise, the results of the moderated-mediation analysis (Figure 2, Table 5) did not support a model of moderated mediation for Instagram use and acne stigma. Interactions of self-compassion on path a (B<0.001, P=.98), path b (B=−0.018, P=.25), or path c (B=0.0004, P=.83) were nonsignificant.

Subsequently, simpler mediation models were explored. Conditional direct and indirect effects of photo-related social media activity on stigmatization were assessed with self-compassion as a covariate. Mediation analysis (Figure 3; Table 6) indicated that there was a significant indirect effect of Facebook photo use on stigmatization via upward appearance comparison (estimation of indirect effect 11.03, SE 5.11, 95% CI 1.19-21.12). There was no significant direct (estimation of direct effect 14.03, SE 13.19, 95% CI −11.86 to 39.93) or total effect (estimation of total effect 25.06, SE 15.14, 95% CI −4.68 to 54.80) of Facebook photo activity on stigmatization. Furthermore, self-compassion predicted lower levels of upward appearance comparison (B=−0.34, P<.001) and stigmatization (B=−0.85, P<.001).

Conversely, mediation analysis (Figure 4; Table 7) indicated that there was no significant direct (estimation of direct effect 0.008, SE 0.013, 95% CI −0.017 to 0.033), total (estimation of total effect 0.008, SE 0.013, 95% CI −0.018 to 0.034), or indirect (estimation of indirect effect 0.0002, SE 0.005, 95% CI −0.011 to 0.009) effect of Instagram use on stigmatization via upward appearance comparison. However, upward appearance comparisons continued to predict stigmatization in Instagram users (B=0.99, P<.001). Self-compassion also continued to predict lower levels of upward appearance comparison (B=−0.33, P<.001) and stigmatization (B=−1.04, P<.001).

Table 4. The conditional direct and indirect effects of Facebook photo function use on stigmatization at values of self-compassion 1 SD below the mean, the mean, and 1 SD above the mean (N=591).
Value of self-compassionDirect effectIndirect effect

B (SE)95% CIB (SE)95% CI
−7.6122.83 (18.17)−12.84 to 58.516.62 (6.92)−8.21 to19.52
0.000014.66 (13.60)−12.05 to 41.3711.11 (5.15)1.02 to 21.25
7.616.49 (15.79)−24.52 to 37.4914.83 (6.21)2.85 to 27.39
Table 5. The conditional direct and indirect effects of Instagram use on stigmatization at values of self-compassion 1 SD below the mean, the mean, and 1 SD above the mean (N=429).
Value of self-compassionDirect effectIndirect effect

B (SE)95% CIB (SE)95% CI
−7.350.005 (0.022)−0.038 to 0.0480.0004 (0.008)−0.018 to 0.015
0.00000.008 (0.01)−0.019 to 0.0350.0005 (0.0056)−0.011 to 0.01
7.350.011 (0.016)−0.021 to 0.0430.0006 (0.0083)−0.021 to 0.12
Table 6. Summary of the mediation analysis for Facebook photo activity (N=591).
VariableB (SE)P value95% CI
Path a: Outcome: UPACSa (R2=0.33, P<.001)

Constant27.07 (2.85)<.00121.47 to 32.67

Facebook photo activity10.07 (4.6).031.02 to 19.11

DACSb0.28 (0.035)<.0010.21 to 0.35

Severity1.36 (0.52).0090.34 to 2.39

Gender3.47 (0.91)<.0011.68 to 5.26

Self-compassion−0.34 (0.042)<.001−0.42 to −0.26

Diagnosis1.42 (0.65).030.014 to 2.7

Long-term health condition−0.73 (0.79).36−2.28 to 0.82
Path b: Outcome: stigma (R2=0.42, P<.001)

Constant29.37 (8.59)<.00112.49 to 46.25

UPACS1.09 (0.12)<.0010.87 to 1.32

Facebook photo use14.03 (13.19).28−11.86 to 39.93

DACS0.25 (0.1).020.046 to 0.45

Severity9.56 (1.54)<.0016.53 to 12.59

Gender−1.45 (2.21).51−5.8 to 2.9

Self-compassion−0.85 (0.12)<.001−1.08 to −0.61

Diagnosis2.5 (1.85).17−1.11 to 6.15

Long-term health condition3.68 (2.18).09−0.6 to 7.96

aUPACS: Upward Appearance Comparison Scale.

bDACS: Downward Appearance Comparison Scale.

Table 7. Summary of the mediation analysis for Instagram use (N=429).
VariableB (SE)P value95% CI
Path a: Outcome: UPACSa (R2=0.30, P<.001)

Constant31.27 (2.98)<.00125.5 to 37.04

Instagram use0.0002 (0.005).98−0.01 to 0.01

DACSb0.25 (0.044)<.0010.16 to 0.33

Severity2.01 (0.59)<.0010.84 to 3.17

Gender3.02 (1.17).100.73 to 5.3

Self-compassion−0.33 (0.048)<.001−0.43 to –0.24

Diagnosis0.8 (0.75).28−0.73 to 2.27

Long-term health condition−0.88 (0.82).29−2.5 to 0.74
Path b: Outcome: stigma (R2=0.39, P<.001)

Constant49.21 (10.13)<.00129.29 to 69.14

UPACS0.99 (0.15)<.0010.7 to 1.29

Instagram use0.008 (0.01).53−0.02 to 0.33

DACS0.1 (0.12).39−0.13 to 0.33

Severity8.83 (1.75)<.0015.38 to 12.27

Gender−1.42 (3.17).65−7.65 to 4.81

Self-compassion−1.04 (0.16)<.001−1.35 to −0.74

Diagnosis4.4 (2.14).040.2 to 8.6

Long-term health condition0.66 (2.58).80−4.4 to 5.72

aUPACS: Upward Appearance Comparison Scale.

bDACS: Downward Appearance Comparison Scale.


This study sought to investigate the relationship between photo-related social media use and feelings of stigmatization in adults with acne. Consistent with the hypothesis, a higher proportion of time engaged in Facebook photo activity, not overall time on Facebook, was correlated with greater feelings of stigmatization in participants with acne, and this relationship was mediated by upward appearance comparison. However, this was the case for Facebook users only, as no such relationship was identified for participants using Instagram; yet among these users, upward appearance comparisons predicted felt stigmatization. Interestingly, although greater self-compassion was related to lower stigmatization, it did not moderate the relationship between social media photo use and acne-related stigma in either Facebook or Instagram users.

It is unclear why there was an indirect relationship between relative Facebook photo activity and stigma but not between Instagram use and stigma. This may reflect the choice of measures, as the measure of Instagram usage did not differentiate between types of usage [32]. Future research should therefore distinguish between types of Instagram use.

Existing research on stigmatization in individuals with skin conditions has primarily focused on stigmatization as a predictor of depression and impaired quality of life, and demographic and condition variables as predictions of stigma. Such research has consistently identified perceived stigmatization as a predictor of reduced quality of life and psychological morbidity. In line with previous research, this study identified associations with stigmatization and gender, perceived severity, and possessing a diagnosis of acne [10,17,20]. However, skin-related comparisons and self-compassion were more consistently associated with felt stigmatization. These findings suggest that the way individuals interact with social media is more important than how long they use it for understanding the associations between social media and well-being. This is important as such meta-cognitive processes are amenable to modification within psychological therapy.

This study has a number of limitations. Clearly, the cross-sectional design prevents comment on causation, and experimental research could usefully investigate the relationship between social comparisons and social media use. Participants for this study were recruited via a web-based platform from a community sample; therefore, information on objective diagnoses and severity was not obtainable, and this prevented the investigation of treatment factors and clinical severity. It is possible that some participants did not have acne and may have had other undiagnosed skin conditions. Also, there were a greater proportion of females in the Instagram group and a higher proportion of people 25 years or older (considered to have adult acne [49]) in the Facebook group, which may have affected the results. However, the majority of individuals with acne tend to self-manage [51], and consequently, the use of a community sample has a number of merits in reaching a wider range of people living with the condition. A final important limitation is that social media use was self-reported, which may have affected the reliability of the data obtained. This could be addressed in future experimental studies.

Nevertheless, the finding that appearance comparisons were associated with stigmatization in both Facebook and Instagram users and mediated the relationship between relative Facebook photo activity and stigmatization provides further support for the important role of skin-specific appearance comparisons in the psychosocial well-being of individuals living with acne, as reported in qualitative research [5,52]. Therefore, the role of upward appearance comparisons on feelings of stigmatization should be considered when working with individuals with acne-related distress. Future research could explore whether this relationship is present in populations with other skin conditions and further explore the relationship with other measures of psychological distress, such as shame, depression, and social anxiety. Given that self-compassion was consistently related to lower levels of stigmatization, interventions based on increasing self-compassion provide an additional avenue for exploring ways of reducing feelings of stigmatization in individuals with acne.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants for volunteering their time and the Medical Humanities Sheffield Network for supporting this research. We would also like to thank Ana Chamorro Coneo for providing feedback on the manuscript. This research was funded by the University of Sheffield as part of the Medical Humanities Sheffield shame and stigma network.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

  1. Liasides J, Apergi FS. Predictors of QOL for adults with acne: the contribution of perceived stigma. 2015 Presented at: The Annual International Conference on Cognitive - Social, and Behavioural Sciences; January 2015; Kyrenia, Cyprus. [CrossRef]
  2. Magin P, Adams J, Heading G, Pond D, Smith W. Psychological sequelae of acne vulgaris: results of a qualitative study. Can Fam Physician 2006;52(8):978-979 [FREE Full text] [Medline]
  3. Roosta N, Black DS, Peng D, Riley LW. Skin disease and stigma in emerging adulthood: impact on healthy development. J Cutan Med Surg 2010;14(6):285-290. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Magin P, Adams J, Heading G, Pond D, Smith W. Experiences of appearance-related teasing and bullying in skin diseases and their psychological sequelae: results of a qualitative study. Scand J Caring Sci 2008;22(3):430-436. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Murray CD, Rhodes K. 'Nobody likes damaged goods': the experience of adult visible acne. Br J Health Psychol 2005;10(Pt 2):183-202. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  6. Grandfield TA, Thompson AR, Turpin G. An attitudinal study of responses to a range of dermatological conditions using the implicit association test. J Health Psychol 2005;10(6):821-829. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Ritvo E, Del Rosso JQ, Stillman MA, La Riche C. Psychosocial judgements and perceptions of adolescents with acne vulgaris: a blinded, controlled comparison of adult and peer evaluations. Biopsychosoc Med 2011;5(1):11 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Timms RM. Moderate acne as a potential barrier to social relationships: myth or reality? Psychol Health Med 2013;18(3):310-320. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  9. Davern J, O'Donnell AT. Stigma predicts health-related quality of life impairment, psychological distress, and somatic symptoms in acne sufferers. PLoS One 2018;13(9):e0205009 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Łakuta P, Marcinkiewicz K, Bergler-Czop B, Brzezińska-Wcisło L. The relationship between psoriasis and depression: a multiple mediation model. Body Image 2016;19:126-132. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Richards HL, Fortune DG, Griffiths CE, Main CJ. The contribution of perceptions of stigmatisation to disability in patients with psoriasis. J Psychosom Res 2001;50(1):11-15. [Medline]
  12. Smith H, Layton AM, Thiboutot D, Smith A, Whitehouse H, Ghumra W, et al. Identifying the impacts of acne and the use of questionnaires to detect these impacts: a systematic literature review. Am J Clin Dermatol 2021;22(2):159-171. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  13. Hassan J, Grogan S, Clark-Carter D, Richards H, Yates VM. The individual health burden of acne: appearance-related distress in male and female adolescents and adults with back, chest and facial acne. J Health Psychol 2009;14(8):1105-1118. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Gudiya, Cecilia MS, Satapathy S, Ramam M. Assessment of body image disturbance, self-esteem and quality of life among adolescents and young adults with acne in a tertiary care facility of India. Indian J Dermatol 2022;67(1):93 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  15. Tan J, Frey MP, Thiboutot D, Layton A, Eady A. Identifying the impacts of acne: a Delphi survey of patients and clinicians. J Cutan Med Surg 2020;24(3):259-266. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  16. Hrehorów E, Salomon J, Matusiak L, Reich A, Szepietowski JC. Patients with psoriasis feel stigmatized. Acta Derm Venereol 2012;92(1):67-72 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Ginsburg IH, Link BG. Feelings of stigmatization in patients with psoriasis. J Am Acad Dermatol 1989;20(1):53-63. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Lu Y, Duller P, van der Valk PG, Evers AW. Helplessness as predictor of stigmatization in patients with psoriasis and atopic dermatitis. Dermatol Psychosom 2003;4(3):146-150. [CrossRef]
  19. Schmid-Ott G, Jaeger B, Kuensebeck HW, Ott R, Lamprecht F. Dimensions of stigmatization in patients with psoriasis in a "questionnaire on experience with skin complaints'. Dermatology 1996;193(4):304-310. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Wittkowski A, Richards HL, Williams J, Fortune DG, Griffiths CEM, Main CJ. An examination of the psychometric properties and factor structure of the feelings of stigmatization questionnaire. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 2007;14(3):248-257. [CrossRef]
  21. Thompson AR. Researching appearance: models, theories and frameworks. In: Rumsey N, Harcourt D, editors. The Oxford Handbook of the Psychology of Appearance. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.
  22. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Sociocultural theory: the media and society. In: Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S, editors. Exacting Beauty: Theory, Assessment, And Treatment Of Body Image Disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999:85-124.
  23. McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA. Sociocultural influences on body image and body changes among adolescent boys and girls. J Soc Psychol 2003;143(1):5-26. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Magin P, Adams J, Heading G, Pond D. 'Perfect skin', the media and patients with skin disease: a qualitative study of patients with acne, psoriasis and atopic eczema. Aust J Prim Health 2011;17(2):181-185. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Adults: media use and attitudes report 2022. Ofcom. 2022.   URL: https:/​/www.​ofcom.org.uk/​__data/​assets/​pdf_file/​0020/​234362/​adults-media-use-and-attitudes-report-2022.​pdf [accessed 2023-02-24]
  26. Collier CN, Harper JC, Cafardi JA, Cantrell WC, Wang W, Foster KW, et al. The prevalence of acne in adults 20 years and older. J Am Acad Dermatol 2008;58(1):56-59. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Spina G, Bozzola E, Ferrara P, Zamperini N, Marino F, Caruso C, et al. Children and adolescent's perception of media device use consequences. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021;18(6):3048 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Holland G, Tiggemann M. A systematic review of the impact of the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image 2016;17:100-110. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Mabe AG, Forney KJ, Keel PK. Do you "like" my photo? Facebook use maintains eating disorder risk. Int J Eat Disord 2014;47(5):516-523. [Medline]
  30. Meier EP, Gray J. Facebook photo activity associated with body image disturbance in adolescent girls. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014;17(4):199-206. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Smith AR, Hames JL, Joiner TE. Status update: maladaptive Facebook usage predicts increases in body dissatisfaction and bulimic symptoms. J Affect Disord 2013;149(1-3):235-240. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Brown Z, Tiggemann M. Attractive celebrity and peer images on Instagram: effect on women's mood and body image. Body Image 2016;19:37-43. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat 1954;7(2):117-140. [CrossRef]
  34. O'Brien KS, Caputi P, Minto R, Peoples G, Hooper C, Kell S, et al. Upward and downward physical appearance comparisons: development of scales and examination of predictive qualities. Body Image 2009 Jun;6(3):201-206. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Myers TA, Crowther JH. Social comparison as a predictor of body dissatisfaction: a meta-analytic review. J Abnorm Psychol 2009;118(4):683-698. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Markham A, Thompson T, Bowling A. Determinants of body-image shame. Pers Individ Dif 2005;38(7):1529-1541. [CrossRef]
  37. Fardouly J, Diedrichs PC, Vartanian LR, Halliwell E. Social comparisons on social media: the impact of Facebook on young women's body image concerns and mood. Body Image 2015;13:38-45. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Fardouly J, Vartanian LR. Negative comparisons about one's appearance mediate the relationship between Facebook usage and body image concerns. Body Image 2015;12:82-88. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Feinstein BA, Hershenberg R, Bhatia V, Latack JA, Meuwly N, Davila J. Negative social comparison on Facebook and depressive symptoms. Psychol Pop Media Cult 2013;2(3):161-170. [CrossRef]
  40. Finlay WM, Lyons E. Social categorizations, social comparisons and stigma: presentations of self in people with learning difficulties. Br J Soc Psychol 2000;39(( Pt 1)):129-146. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Kellett S, Gilbert P. Acne: a biopsychosocial and evolutionary perspective with a focus on shame. Br J Health Psychol 2001;6(Pt 1):1-24. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Neff KD. The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self Identity 2003;2(3):223-250. [CrossRef]
  43. Hilbert A, Braehler E, Schmidt R, Löwe B, Häuser W, Zenger M. Self-compassion as a resource in the self-stigma process of overweight and obese individuals. Obes Facts 2015;8(5):293-301 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Wong CCY, Mak WWS, Liao KYH. Self-compassion: a potential buffer against affiliate stigma experienced by parents of children with autism spectrum disorders. Mindfulness 2016;7(6):1385-1395. [CrossRef]
  45. Kelly AC, Zuroff DC, Shapira LB. Soothing oneself and resisting self-attacks: the treatment of two intrapersonal deficits in depression vulnerability. Cogn Ther Res 2009;33(3):301-313. [CrossRef]
  46. Motley RJ, Finlay AY. Practical use of a disability index in the routine management of acne. Clin Exp Dermatol 1992;17(1):1-3. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Raes F, Pommier E, Neff KD, Van Gucht D. Construction and factorial validation of a short form of the self-compassion scale. Clin Psychol Psychother 2011;18(3):250-255. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York City: Guilford Press; 2017.
  49. Khunger N, Kumar C. A clinico-epidemiological study of adult acne: is it different from adolescent acne? Indian J Dermatol Venereol Leprol 2012;78(3):335-341 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Böhm D, Schwanitz P, Stock Gissendanner S, Schmid-Ott G, Schulz W. Symptom severity and psychological sequelae in rosacea: results of a survey. Psychol Health Med 2014;19(5):586-591. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  51. Vilar GN, Santos LA, Sobral Filho JF. Quality of life, self-esteem and psychosocial factors in adolescents with acne vulgaris. An Bras Dermatol 2015;90(5):622-629 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Prior J, Khadaroo A. 'I sort of balance it out'. Living with facial acne in emerging adulthood. J Health Psychol 2015;20(9):1154-1165. [CrossRef] [Medline]


DACS: Downward Appearance Comparison Scale
UPACS: Upward Appearance Comparison Scale


Edited by R Dellavalle; submitted 27.12.22; peer-reviewed by T Simonart, A Layton; comments to author 08.02.23; revised version received 16.02.23; accepted 19.02.23; published 12.04.23

Copyright

©Kate Adkins, Paul G Overton, Julia Moses, Andrew Thompson. Originally published in JMIR Dermatology (http://derma.jmir.org), 12.04.2023.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Dermatology, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on http://derma.jmir.org, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.